Close

Results 1 to 1 of 1
  1. #1
    Stranger
    Join Date
    Oct 2005
    Posts
    2
    Rep Power
    0

    Default Human pheromones and sexual attraction from medical journal 2004

    visit-red-300x50PNG
    Review






    Human pheromones and

    sexual attraction






    Karl

    Grammer
    a,

    Bernhard

    Fink
    a[font=AdvP41153C],[/

    font]*, Nick

    Neave
    b







    a
    Ludwig–Boltzmann-Institute for Urban Ethology, c/o

    Institute of Anthropology, University of Vienna, Althanstrasse 14, A-1090 Vienna,

    Austria







    b
    H

    uman Cognitive Neuroscience Unit, School of Psychology and Sport Sciences, Northumbria University, Newcastle upon

    Tyne, NE1 8ST, UK







    Received 30 April 2004;

    accepted 19 August

    2004






    Abstract






    [/font

    ]
    Olfactory communication is very common amongst animals, and since the discovery of

    an accessory olfactory system in humans, possible


    human olfactory communication has gained considerable

    scientific interest. The importance of the human sense of smell has by far been

    underestimated in the past.

    Humans and other primates have been regarded as primarily ‘optical animals’ with highly developed powers of



    vision but a relatively undeveloped sense of smell. In recent years this assumption has undergone major revision.

    Several studies indicate that

    humans indeed seem to use olfactory communication and are even able to produce and

    perceive certain pheromones; recent studies have found

    that pheromones may play an important role in the

    behavioural and reproduction biology of humans. In this article we review the present

    evidence of the effect of

    human pheromones and discuss the role of olfactory cues in human sexual

    behaviour.





    #

    2004 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights

    reserved.







    Keywords:

    Pheromone; Human; Sexual attraction; Mate preferences; Menstrual cycle; Oral

    contraception







    1.

    Introduction






    The importance of pheromones in

    intra-species communication


    has long been known in insects. A classical example

    is bombykol, the sexual

    attractant of the butterfly
    Bombyx



    mori
    . Bombykol is produced by the female butterflies in

    odour glands of

    the abdomen. Male butterflies detect the

    pheromone with sensory cells, located in the antennae and

    can find

    the females by the gradient of her odour. As little as

    one molecule of bombykol is enough to stimulate the



    receptor cells and facilitate the orientation reaction. Several

    studies suggest that pheromones play an

    important role also

    in mammalian social behaviour and thus in humans as

    well.








    www.elsevier.com/locate/ejogrb



    European Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology and

    Reproductive Biology

    118 (2005) 135–142

    * Corresponding author. Tel.: +43 1 4277 54769; fax: +43 1 4277

    9547.










    0301-2115/$ – see front matter
    # 2004

    Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.



    doi:10.1016/j.ejogrb.2004.08.010






    The present

    article reviews the current evidence how


    pheromones

    in
    fluence human life and interactions

    and

    discusses the consequences for human sexual attraction and



    mate-choice.






    1.1.

    Smell






    According toKohl et al.

    [1] the sense

    of smell has largely


    been underestimated in reproductive behaviours and it has

    long been assumed that

    humans are

    microsmatic(poor

    smellers) and rely essentially on visual and verbal

    cues

    when assessing potential mates. Certainly visual stimuli

    play a key role in the perceptions of others

    within a

    sociosexual context, especially at a distance, but when

    individuals get closer and personal

    intimacy is increased, it

    is likely that smell also plays a key role a variety of

    sociosexual behaviours.

    Recent studies have indeed

    suggested that olfaction (conscious and unconscious)

    can play a

    signi
    ficant role in human reproductive

    biology.

    Zajonc
    s

    [2]

    affective

    primacy
    hypothesis states that both



    positive and negative affect can be evoked with minimal

    stimulus input and only minor cognitive involvement.



    Olfactory signals induce emotional responses even if an

    olfactory stimulus is not consciously perceived: this

    is due

    to the fact that olfactory receptors not only send projections

    to the neocortex for conscious

    processing (e.g. the nature of

    a particular aroma) but also to the limbic system for

    emotional processing

    (e.g. memories and affect associated

    with a particular

    smell).






    1.2.

    Pheromones






    The term

    pheromonewas introduced by Karlson and


    Luscher

    [3] and it

    derives from the Greek words

    pherein






    (to carry) and

    hormon

    (to excite). Pheromones are referred


    to as

    ecto-hormonesas they are chemical messengers that

    are emitted into

    the environment from the body where they

    can then activate

    speci
    fic physiological or

    behavioural

    responses in other individuals of the same species.

    According to McClintock

    [4] pheromones

    can be divided

    into two classes. Firstly,

    signal

    pheromones
    produce shortterm



    behavioural changes and seem to act as attractants and

    repellents. Secondly,

    primer

    pheromones
    produce longerlasting



    changes in behaviour via their activation of the



    hypothalamic
    pituitary

    adrenal (HPA) axis

    [4]. In

    particular,

    it is assumed that primer pheromones trigger the

    secretion of GnRH from the hypothalamus, which

    in turn

    triggers the release of gonadotropins (LH, FSH) from the

    pituitary gland. These gonadotropins

    in
    fluence gonadal

    hormone

    secretion, e.g. follicle maturation in the ovaries in

    females, testosterone and sperm production in males. In



    support, in various species the short-term exposures of

    females to males have been associated with a

    corresponding

    rise in testosterone

    [5]. Four

    speci
    fic functions of

    pheromones

    have been determined: opposite-sex attractants,

    same-sex repellents,

    mother
    infant bonding attractants

    and

    menstrual cycle modulators

    [6]. It is the

    first category that

    this review

    will focus upon though may draw upon evidence

    from the other categories wherever

    relevant.






    1.3. Pheromone

    detection






    In most mammals, a specialised region

    of the olfactory


    system called the vomeronasal organ (VNO), also referred to

    as

    Jacobsons organ

    is responsible for pheromone

    detection. The principal evidence that the

    VNO plays a

    role in mammalian pheromone detection comes from lesion

    studies where removal of the VNO

    produces reliable

    impairments in reproductive behaviours

    [7]. The VNO

    is

    located above the hard palate on both sides of the nasal

    septum and it is lined with receptor cells whose

    axons

    project to the accessory olfactory bulb, which sends its

    projects to the hypothalamic nuclei

    [8]. Pheromones

    can

    thus potentially in
    fluence

    sexual and reproductive behaviours

    and endocrine function via the HPA axis

    [9]. There



    has been some scepticism concerning the ability of humans

    to detect and respond to pheromones due to the facts

    that

    VNO appears to vestigial in some primates, and the

    accessory olfactory bulb is not discernable in

    humans
    [9].



    However, it has since been reported that humans do

    possess a functional VNO that responds to pheromones



    (even in picogram amounts) in a

    sex-speci
    fic

    manner






    [10
    [fon

    t=AdvP41153C]
    12][/size

    ]
    . Recently, the

    identi
    fication of a pheromone




    receptor gene expressed in human olfactory mucosa has

    further strengthened the case for a functioning VNO

    [13].



    Further evidence comes from patients with

    Kallmann
    s

    syndrome, which occurs

    due to the underdevelopment of

    the olfactory bulb in the embryo and minimal GnRH

    secretions from the

    hypothalamus. Individuals have

    underdeveloped gonads, lack secondary sexual characteristics,

    are anosmic,

    and preliminary research indicates that

    they show no response to pheromones (personal communication

    cited in

    [1]).

    [/left

    ]




    1.4. Pheromone

    production






    The main producers of human pheromones

    are the


    apocrine glands located in the axillae and pubic region. The

    high concentration of apocrine glands

    found in the armpits

    led to the term

    axillary

    organ
    , which is considered an



    independent

    organ[

    size=2]’
    of human odour production. Apocrine

    glands develop in the

    embryo, but become functional only

    with the onset of puberty. At sexual maturation, they

    produce steroidal

    secretions derived from 16-androstenes

    (androstenone and androstenol) via testosterone, and as

    such, the

    concentrations of several 16-androstenes is



    signi
    ficantly higher in males

    [14]. Freshly

    produced

    apocrine secretions are odourless but are transformed into

    the odorous androstenone and androstenol

    by aerobic

    coryeform bacteria

    [15]. In the

    vagina, aliphatic acids

    (referred to as copulins) are secreted and their odour varies

    with the menstrual

    cycle
    [16]. It

    is now possible to isolate






    K. Grammer

    136 et al. / European Journal of

    Obstetrics & Gynecology and Reproductive Biology 118 (2005)

    135
    [font=AdvP4B2E3F]142







    [

    /font]
    and manufacture synthetic human pheromones and such


    compounds are often

    used in research as they are relatively

    easy to make, convenient to store, and easy to

    apply.





    1.5. Pheromone effects on animal

    reproductive


    behaviours





    Preliminary studies

    in the 1960s demonstrated that


    exposure to boar odour elicited the mating stance in females.

    Subsequent

    experiments showed that application of male

    urine or semen to the

    female
    s snout also produced the

    same

    effect. Studies have appeared to demonstrate a number of



    con
    firmed effects of pheromones in

    animals. Firstly

    the






    Lee-Boo

    t Effect
    [17]

    describes the effects of the social


    environment on the female

    reproductive cycle. The authors

    noted that when female mice were housed 4 in a cage their

    oestrous cycles

    became synchronised and extended.

    Secondly, the

    Whitten

    effect
    [18]

    confirmed that female

    mice housed together displayed an extended oestrous cycle,

    but further noted

    that when a male was introduced the

    females ovulated synchronously

    3
    4 days later. The

    substance was

    found to be androgen-based pheromones

    secreted in the

    male
    s urine.

    Thirdly, the

    Bruce

    effect
    [19]

    describes the effect of

    housing pregnant mice with males that

    were not their

    original mates. Within 48 h of such pairings,

    signi
    ficantly

    more miscarriages

    were observed in the females. Subsequent

    mating with the new male within

    3
    6 days then always

    followed the

    failed pregnancy. The inclusion of castrated or

    juvenile male strangers had no such effects. This appears to



    be a male tactic of blocking the pregnancy by a previous

    male and bringing the female quickly into oestrous.

    Finally

    the
    Vandenburgh

    effect
    [20]

    notes that young female rats

    exposed to adult males for 20 days

    after weaning entered

    puberty earlier than female pups not exposed to males. Male

    pheromones stimulate

    puberty, probably by releasing LH,

    which stimulates follicular growth, presumably so that they

    can mate

    earlier. A related effect was noted in that female

    mice housed alone attain puberty earlier than female mice



    housed together, females can thus delay puberty in their



    conspeci
    fics, probably by

    suppressing LH and FSH release

    from the anterior pituitary

    gland.






    1.6. Pheromones and human reproductive

    behaviours






    Several authors have speculated that

    pheromones may


    in
    fluence human

    sociosexual behaviours (e.g.

    [21,22]) and



    evidence for the effects of putative pheromones on human

    sexual behaviours has come from several sources:



    1. Human correlates of animal effects

    McClintock
    [23]

    reported that human female college

    students demonstrated

    synchrony in their menstrual

    cycles when housed in shared accommodation

    (Lee
    Boot

    effect). Preti et al.

    [24] extended

    this research by applying

    extracts of female sweat to the upper lips of female

    volunteers three times per

    week for 4 months. At the end

    of this time the participants showed

    signi
    ficantly greater

    menstrual

    synchrony than volunteers in a control group.

    Cutler et al.

    [25] also

    showed that the application of male

    axillary secretions to the upper lips of female volunteers

    also had a

    regulatory effect on the menstrual cycle

    (Whitten effect). Ellis and Garber

    [26] showed

    that girls

    in stepfather-present homes experienced faster puberty

    than girls in single-mother homes, the

    younger the

    daughter when the new male arrived on the scene then the

    earlier her pubertal maturation

    (Vandenburgh effect).

    2. Laboratory studies

    In an early report, Kirk-Smith et al.

    [27] asked 12

    male

    and female undergraduates to rate photographs of people,

    animals and buildings using 159-point bipolar

    scales (e.g.



    unattractive
    attractive), while

    wearing surgical masks

    either impregnated with androstenol or left undoctored.

    Mood ratings were also

    completed. In the presence of

    androstenol, male and female stimuli were also rated as

    being

    warmer

    and

    more

    friendly
    . Van Toller et al.

    [28]






    showed that skin conductance in volunteers exposed to


    androstenone was higher than that of

    non-exposed

    volunteers thereby providing evidence as to the

    physiological effects of pheromone exposure.

    However,

    Benton and Wastell
    [29]

    had groups of females read

    either a neutral or a sexually

    arousing passage whilst

    exposed to either androstenol or a placebo substance.

    While sexual arousal was

    higher in the

    arousal






    condition, the authors found no evidence

    that exposure


    to androstenol had

    in
    fluenced sexual feelings.



    Filsinger et al.
    [30]

    asked males and females to rate

    vignettes of a

    fictional target male and female

    using

    semantic differentials, and also to provide a selfassessment

    of mood. The test materials had been

    sealed

    into plastic bags, which were either impregnated with

    androstenol, androstenone, a synthetic musk

    control, and

    a no-odour control. Females exposed to androstenone

    produced lower sexual attractiveness

    ratings of the target

    male, while males exposed to androstenol perceived the

    male targets to be more

    sexually attractive.

    The interpretation from such studies is further

    complicated by two factors. Firstly,

    female olfactory

    sensitivity is moderated by the menstrual cycle with

    smell sensitivity peaking at ovulation

    [31]. Benton

    [32]






    reported that androstenol application

    in
    fluenced ratings


    of subjective

    mood at ovulation, and Grammer
    [21]

    found

    that females rated androstenone differently at various



    phases of their menstrual cycle. Secondly, the use of oral

    contraception may affect smell sensitivity and

    gonadal

    hormone levels thereby possibly disrupting pheromone

    detection. Use of the contraceptive pill does

    indeed

    appear to in
    fluence female

    perception of

    androstenone






    [font=AdvP41153C]
    [21]
    [/fo

    nt].


    More recently Thorne et al.

    [33] employed a

    repeatedmeasures,

    double blind, balanced crossover design to

    assess the possible

    in
    fluence of menstrual cycle

    phase






    K. Grammer et al. / European Journal of

    Obstetrics & Gynecology and Reproductive Biology 118 (2005)

    135
    142

    137







    and

    contraceptive pill use. Sixteen pill and non-pill users


    were tested during both menses and mid-cycle in both



    pheromone-present and pheromone-absent conditions.

    During each session (four in all) the volunteers rated

    male

    vignette characters, and photographs of male faces, on

    various aspects of attractiveness. Pheromone

    exposure

    resulted in signi
    ficantly

    higher attractiveness ratings of

    vignette characters, and faces. Use of the contraceptive

    pill or menstrual

    cycle phase had equivocal effects on

    some vignette items but neither had any

    in
    fluence on

    female ratings of

    male facial attractiveness.

    Not all laboratory studies have found positive results

    however (e.g.

    [34]), and some

    authors are sceptical that

    higher primate reproductive behaviours are

    signi
    ficantly



    in
    fluenced by pheromones

    [35]. Thus,

    while the current

    scienti
    fic

    opinion regarding the existence of human

    pheromones remains positive, opinion remains divided as

    to whether

    such substances do in fact in
    fluence

    human

    sociosexual behaviours. This is probably due to the fact

    that while a wealth of laboratory-based

    studies has been

    conducted, very different methodologies mean that

    comparisons between studies are

    dif
    ficult. Furthermore,



    methodologically solid double blind, placebo-controlled,

    crossover studies are few and far between, the Thorne

    et

    al.
    [33]

    study being an exception. However, that study

    was laboratory

    based and simply required participants to

    rate the attractiveness of hypothetical opposite-sex

    characters

    based on written descriptions and photographs.

    The ecological validity of such laboratory-based

    studies is

    therefore questionable.

    3. Real-life studies

    While laboratory studies are able to exert more control



    over the varying factors involved, of potential greater

    relevance are studies assessing the effects of

    pheromones

    in real-life situations. Early studies were, however, not

    promising. For example, Morris and Udry

    [36] prepared



    aliphatic acid smears, formulated to mimic concentrations

    shown to be effective in enhancing monkey



    reproductive behaviour. The solution was smeared on

    the chests of 62 married women on eight randomly



    assigned nights through three menstrual cycles. Volunteers

    did not report any increase in sexual intercourse

    on

    these test nights. However, Cowley and Brooksbank

    [37]






    asked males and females to wear a necklace either


    containing an opposite-sex pheromone or a

    control

    substance while they slept. The next day, they found that

    women who had worn the male pheromones in

    their

    necklace reported

    signi
    ficantly more interactions with



    males than the control group.

    Two studies which have often been cited as the

    strongest evidence yet

    provided for the in
    fluence of



    pheromones on human sociosexual behaviour are those

    of Cutler et al.

    [38] and McCoy

    and Pitino

    [39]. Both



    studies employed double blind, placebo-controlled

    methods and focussed upon the effects of synthetic



    pheromones on self-reported sociosexual behaviours in

    young men

    [38] and women

    [39]. In the

    first study

    [38] 38



    male volunteers recorded the occurrence of six sociosexual

    behaviours (petting/affection/kissing; formal



    dates; informal dates; sleeping next to a partner; sexual

    intercourse; and masturbation) over a 2-week

    baseline






    period. Over the next 6 weeks the

    volunteers kept the


    same records while daily applying a male pheromone or a

    control substance added to

    their usual aftershave lotion.

    The authors reported that a

    signi
    ficantly higher proportion

    of

    pheromone users compared to placebo users

    showed an increase from baseline in

    sexual

    intercourse







    [left

    ]and
    sleeping next to a romantic

    partner
    . In general 58%


    of the

    pheromone group compared to 19% of the placebo

    group showed increases in two or more behaviours

    compared to

    baseline; 41% of the pheromone group

    compared to 9.5% of the placebo group showed increases

    in three or more

    behaviours compared to baseline.

    In the second study
    [39]

    36 female volunteers recorded

    the occurrence of the same six

    socio-sexual behaviours and

    an additional behaviour

    male

    approaches
    over a

    2-week






    base

    line
    period. Over the next 6 weeks

    they then either


    applied a synthetic female pheromone or a control

    substance added to their usual perfume

    on a daily basis.

    While the groups did not differ in their sociosexual

    behaviours at baseline, a

    signi
    ficantly higher proportion of



    the pheromone group showed increases in the following

    behaviours:

    sexual

    intercourse
    ,

    sleeping next to a



    partner
    ,

    formal

    dates
    and

    [font=AdvP41153C]petting/affection/kissing[/fon

    t].

    However, as pheromone exposure can shift

    the timing of

    ovulation, the authors recalculated the data to only include

    the

    first experimental cycle. After these

    recalculations the

    pheromone group only

    signi
    ficantly differed from the



    placebo group in
    sexual

    intercourse
    and

    formal

    dating
    .

    In terms of percentages,

    three or more sociosexual

    behaviours increased over baseline in 74% of pheromone

    users but only 23% of

    placebo users. As there was no

    increase in self-reported masturbation the authors argued

    that the changes

    did not re
    flect changes in sexual



    motivation, but that the pheromones had

    ‘‘positive sexual

    attractant

    effects
    . . .’’

    (p. 374).

    The results of these studies appear to provide

    impressive

    evidence for the effects of synthetic pheromones

    on sexual attractiveness. However, there are a

    number of

    methodological problems with the studies,

    which make the

    findings less emphatic. Firstly, the



    studies did not control for the attractiveness of the

    volunteers nor make allowance for this when allocating



    the conditions. If for example the pheromone groups had

    contained slightly more attractive individuals than

    the

    control groups, then subsequent positive effects attributed

    to pheromones may be misleading. Secondly,

    all the

    data were of the self-report kind (prone to error and

    subjective bias especially as

    back[s

    ize=2]fi[/size]
    lling

    was allowed in

    the second study) and as such no objective record of the



    putative effects of pheromone versus placebo were

    obtained. Thirdly the groups differed widely in terms

    of






    K. Grammer

    138 et al. / European Journal of

    Obstetrics & Gynecology and Reproductive Biology 118 (2005)

    135
    [font=AdvP4B2E3F]142







    [

    /font]
    their dating status with some being married, some in


    long-term

    relationships and others being single. Those in

    relationships would have certainly recorded more of

    certain

    sociosexual behaviours than the single volunteers,

    it would have been better if the entire subject pool were



    single males seeking more dating/sex opportunities.

    Fourthly, the baseline period of 2 weeks is

    dif
    ficult to

    equate with a testing

    period of 6 weeks even though

    average differences from baseline were analysed. How

    can we be sure that the

    social behaviour of the volunteers

    changed not as a result of pheromone exposure but by

    other factors during

    the experimental period, e.g. going

    on holiday, celebrating at an

    of
    fice party? While the

    actual

    behaviours were recorded, the context within

    which those behaviours occurred was not controlled for.

    The

    evidence from these two studies thus indicates

    that certain sociosexual behaviours are increased in

    males

    and females who wear pheromones, compared to

    baseline. However, the studies do not convincingly show

    that

    the pheromone and placebo groups were well

    matched; that the baseline and experimental conditions

    were

    matched in terms of various temporal and

    behavioural factors; that objective changes in sociosexual



    behaviours did occur; and that the pheromones served as

    a

    sexual

    attractants
    rather than say a mood

    enhancer,

    con
    fidence builder,

    etc.

    4. Genetic signalling

    Various

    good

    genes
    theories of sexual selection

    have

    emphasised the importance of

    immunocompetence






    [40,41]

    in that females can obtain good genes for their


    offspring by

    mating with males whose genes are

    complementary to their own. A possible mechanism

    by which this can be

    achieved is via body odour. The

    major histocompatibility complex (MHC) is a large

    chromosomal region

    containing closely linked polymorphic

    genes that play a role in immunological self/

    non-self recognition;

    this genetic information is relayed

    by androgen-based pheromones

    [42]. Numerous

    studies

    in rodents have now established that MHC genotype is

    involved in odour production, and such odours

    are used in

    individual discrimination

    [43]. House

    mice learn the

    MHC identity of their family during development and

    avoid mating with individuals carrying

    familial MHC

    genes; they do so through the use of odour cues from

    urine (e.g.

    [44,45]). Is

    there any evidence that humans

    possess these abilities?

    Some studies have shown that women seem to prefer



    the odours of immunocompatible men. Wedekind et

    al.






    [46]

    HLA-typed (Human Leukocyte Antigen is the


    human MHC) 49 women

    and 44 men and asked the

    women to rate the attractiveness of the odours of t-shirts

    worn by three

    MHC-similar and three MHC-dissimilar

    men.Women rated the odour of the MHC-dissimilar men

    as

    more

    pleasant
    , and this odour was

    signi
    ficantly more

    likely to

    remind them of their own mate
    s

    odour.

    Interestingly, the preferences of women taking an oral

    contraceptive were

    reversed
    they preferred the

    MHCsimilar

    odours. This could be due to the fact that oral

    contraceptives mimic the effects of pregnancy,

    and

    pregnant females may be attracted to MHC-similar

    individuals who are likely to be close kin and

    potential

    reproductive helpers.

    In a similar study, Thornhill and Gangstad

    [47]






    measured bilateral physical traits in males and females


    and then asked the volunteers to wear

    the same T-shirt for

    two consecutive nights. Opposite-sex participants then

    rated the shirts for

    pleasantness,

    sexiness

    and








    intensity[siz

    e=2]; donor[/size]
    s facial attractiveness

    was also


    assessed by different opposite-sex volunteers. Non-pill

    users in the fertile phase of their

    menstrual cycle gave the

    T-shirts worn by symmetrical males higher ratings; this

    was not seen in females

    using the contraceptive pill, or in

    females at unfertile phases of their cycle. Female

    symmetry had no

    in
    fluence on male ratings. The

    authors

    proposed that the so-called

    scent of

    symmetry
    is an

    honest indicator

    of male genetic quality.

    In a real-life study of actual mate choices, Ober et

    al.






    [48]

    found evidence for HLA-dependent mate preferences


    in a

    population of Hutterites (a small, genetically

    isolated religious sect). They found that couples were less



    likely to share MHC haplotypes than chance, and in

    couples that had a similar MHC they demonstrated



    unusually long inter-birth intervals (unconscious avoidance

    of inbreeding?).

    Milinski and Wedekind

    [49] HLA-typed

    males and

    females and then asked them to smell 36 scents

    commonly used in perfume/aftershave. They rated

    each

    scent on whether they liked it or not, and whether they

    would use it on themselves. The authors

    reported a

    signi
    ficant correlation

    between HLA and scent scoring

    for themselves but not for others, showing the people

    unconsciously select

    perfumes to enhance their own body

    odours that reveal their genetic

    make-up.






    1.7. Pheromones and the battle of the

    sexes






    Differential parental investment theory

    [50] predicts

    that


    when looking for long-term relationships females should

    seek out and choose males who are ready to

    invest resources

    in their offspring. This minimizes female investment, but

    maximizes overall investment

    through added male assistance.

    In contrast, males are expected either to attempt

    copulation frequently and

    with as many fertile females as

    possible, or to develop a long-term pair bond. This helps to

    ensure that

    either a large number of offspring survive

    without

    signi
    ficant paternal investment, or

    that male parental

    investment occurs primarily when another male does not

    father offspring.

    According to

    this theory, it is adaptive for females and

    males to develop and use cognition in mate selection, which



    takes into account biological constraints. Thus, mate

    selection is a task of information processing, and

    evolution






    K. Grammer et al. / European Journal of

    Obstetrics & Gynecology and Reproductive Biology 118 (2005)

    135
    142

    139







    would

    have favoured individuals who were able to quickly


    and reliably process information that allowed them to

    make

    appropriate mating decisions. Adaptive cognition could be

    expected to lead to optimal decision-making

    under a wide

    spectrum of socio-economic constraints. The existence of

    ubiquitous sex

    speci
    fic differences in mate selection

    criteria






    [51]

    attests that male and female cognition is adapted to the




    biological constraints of mate selection.

    Neither males nor females can perceive ovulation in

    humans

    consciously. This is surprising in the light of the fact

    that it has been shown to be associated with a number

    of

    overt physiological and behavioural changes. One

    unconscious






    mechanism associated with these

    menstrual cycle


    changes might be that of olfactory perceptions.

    Alexander and Noonan

    [52], and

    Symons
    [53]

    have

    argued that hidden oestrous has evolved because females



    need to trick males into forming a bond. Males unaware of

    female


    s fertility would remain bonded to ensure

    impregnation and paternity. A

    female providing clues to

    her ovulation might risk losing male investment, due to

    paternal uncertainty and

    the limited temporal reproductive

    interaction. This development would implicate the male fear

    of cuckoldry

    as an evolutionary pressure

    [50]. The

    outcome

    would be that the female
    s

    ability to secure paternal care is

    affected by mechanisms that increase temporal aspects of

    the pair bond

    and enhance male con
    fidence of

    paternity.

    In contrast with this line of argument, Benshoff and

    Thornhill

    [54] and Symons

    [53] have

    proposed a second

    evolutionary scenario in which hidden oestrous evolved to

    increase the chances of

    successful cuckoldry by females so

    they

    ‘‘can escape the negative consequences

    of being pawns

    in marriage games
    ’’

    ([55]

    p. 350). Once monogamy is

    established, a

    female
    s best strategy would be to

    copulate

    outside the pair bond because she can then obtain superior

    genes with a certain expectation of

    paternal investment. In

    this case the outcome is genetically superior offspring.

    These two hypotheses imply

    different impacts of

    heritable traits. If those genes which induce paternal care

    were relevant for offspring

    success, a male paternitysecuring

    function for lost oestrous would be possible. If

    there are other relevant

    traits not related to paternal care but

    relevant to offspring survival, then hidden oestrous could

    allow

    females to exploit occasional opportunities to mate

    outside the pair bond

    [56]. In both

    cases, male knowledge of

    ovulation may be selected against because it would hinder

    the

    female
    s mating strategies

    [52,57].



    Recently, the second hypothesis has received considerable

    support. Bellis and Baker

    [58] conducted

    a study of

    2708 females and found those 13.8% of 145

    unprotected






    extra-pair copulations (EPC)

    occurred during the fertile


    period and were preceded in most cases by intra-pair

    copulations (IPC). EPCs

    were rarely followed by IPCs.

    According to his study EPC and thus female

    in
    fidelity peaks

    at ovulation. The

    authors conclude that these results hint at

    female-induced sperm competition, which would be

    expected by the

    second hypothesis of the evolutionary

    function of concealed ovulation discussed above. Still it is

    unclear

    what proximate mechanism or mechanisms cue

    female EPC at ovulation. The assumption that concealed

    ovulation

    serves to deceive males is common to all these

    theories. Supposedly, females deceive males about the fertile



    phase of the menstrual cycle to help ensure male parental

    investment, which yields an optimal number of

    offspring.

    Additionally, concealed ovulation helps females to monopolize

    reproduction and, as a consequence,

    forces males to

    develop reproductive strategies for gaining access to

    ovulating females. It is reasonable to

    expect male counter

    strategies would develop against the deceptive attempt by

    females to conceal ovulation.

    Grammer
    [21]

    described a

    possible male counter strategy: the evolution of

    the

    androstenone
    androstenol

    signalling system. In his study,

    290 female subjects rated the odour of androstenone. A

    change in assessment

    throughout the menstrual cycle was

    found: at the time of ovulation the women found the scent of



    androstenone, the most dominant odour of the male armpit,

    to be more pleasant than on the other days of the

    menstrual

    cycle. These results suggest that there is a change in the

    emotional evaluation of males triggered

    by the reaction to

    androstenone. The

    findings support previous results by



    Maiworm
    [59],

    which were of borderline signi
    ficance.

    Male

    body odour is usually perceived as unattractive and

    unpleasant by females but this evaluation changes

    at the

    point in the menstrual cycle when conception is most likely.

    This

    finding is underlined by the fact that

    anosmia to

    androstenone also varies with cycle. At the conceptual

    optimum we

    find fewer anosmic females. It could

    be

    suggested that changes in anosmia during the cycle could

    also be a female strategy, although more data

    need to be

    gathered to prove this hypothesis. Thus the change in female

    attitudes towards male body odour

    could have a strong

    impact on mate selection and perhaps self-initiated

    copulations by females. If we regard

    the

    androstenol







    [left

    ]androstenone-signaling system, the situation for androstenol


    seems clear, it makes males more attractive for

    females.

    Female advantage in this case is nil, unless

    fitter males

    produce more

    androstenol. The situation is more complicated

    because producing androstenol inevitably produces



    androstenone. The androstenone production has a disadvantage

    in its unpleasantness. Hence

    attractiveness-enhancing

    androstenol immediately oxidizes to androstenone,

    which repels females. A

    non-producing male could do quite

    well in a population of producers, because females would

    not be repelled

    by his body odour. So the attractivenessenhancing

    component of the smell does not seem to be the

    main, or at

    least only, function of the signalling system.

    Regarding androstenone, the fact that females assessed its



    odour as more pleasant at the time of ovulation could be of

    advantage for males, as odorous males will be

    more

    successful when approaching ovulating females, rather than

    non-ovulating females. This suggests that

    males use a kind

    of passive

    ovulation-radarfor the detection of the actually

    hidden

    ovulation.

    [/left]




    K. Grammer

    140 et al. / European Journal of

    Obstetrics & Gynecology and Reproductive Biology 118 (2005)

    135
    [font=AdvP4B2E3F]142







    [

    /font]
    Females faced with an evolved male strategy to detect


    hidden ovulation

    would be likely to develop a counter

    strategy. One possible strategy could be to manipulate male

    cognition

    and thus adaptive male information processing in

    mate selection. Research on many species of non-human



    primates (especially on rhesus monkeys) has shown the

    ability to perceive ovulation by smell. Although

    normally

    motivated to copulate, when sexually inexperienced rhesus

    males were made anosmic they showed no

    further sexual

    motivation despite a powerful visual cue: the

    female
    s

    swelling

    [60].

    Furthermore, rhesus males show no interest in

    ovariectomized rhesus females, presumably because ovariectomized



    rhesus females lose the odour characteristic of

    ovulation. Rhesus males regain interest in copulation when



    the vaginal secretions from non-ovariectomized females are

    applied to ovariectomized females. Studies on

    menstrual

    cycle
    fluctuations in

    the fatty-acid composition of

    women
    s

    vaginal

    fluids indicated that a similar type

    of signalling

    system might also exist in humans
    [16,

    61
    [col

    or=#000066]63][/color]
    . For example,

    human vaginal secretions have a

    composition that is similar

    to the vaginal secretions of female rhesus monkeys. The

    application to

    ovariectomized female rhesus monkeys, either

    of human, or rhesus vaginal secretions, induced similar



    activation of rhesus male sexual interest

    [64].

    The

    behaviourally active fraction of the rhesus vaginal



    secretions
    referred to as

    Copulins’—consists of volatile,

    short-chained fatty acids

    [65]. These

    same substances (i.e.,

    the short-chained fatty acids: acetic, propanoic, butanoic,

    methylpropanoic,

    methylbutanoic, methylpentanoic acid)

    occur in human vaginal secretions, albeit in slightly different



    amounts
    [16].

    In addition, the composition of these copulins

    varies during the menstrual cycle

    [62].



    Cowley et al.
    [66]

    found that rhesus vaginal secretions

    change

    peoples
    assessment of other people,

    and that the

    application of copulins tends to yield a more positive

    impression of females. Doty et al.

    [67] used a

    questionnaire

    to evaluate the intensity and pleasantness of different

    vaginal






    fl
    ui

    ds from a complete menstrual cycle. They found that


    odour at ovulation was both the most intense odour and

    the

    least unpleasant.

    In a study by Juette (unpublished data) synthesized

    female vaginal secretions

    (
    Copulins) were tested for their

    ability to act as signals for males.

    Menstrual, ovulatory and

    pre-menstrual fatty acid compositions of Copulins and an

    odourless water control

    were presented to 60 non-smoking

    male subjects for 25 min in a double-blind experiment. To

    control for

    changes in sex hormones that were induced by

    copulins, saliva-samples were taken before and after

    presentation.

    While inhaling either a composition of copulins or a

    control, males rated pictures of females

    for attractiveness. It

    was shown that ovulatory fatty acid compositions stimulated

    male androgen secretion

    and changed the discriminatory

    cognitive capacities of males with regard to female attractiveness

    in that

    males became less discriminating. As we can

    learn from the above examples, human pheromones seem to

    work as

    beautifully balanced
    strategic

    weapons
    in

    the






    battle

    of the sexes
    and the

    war of

    signals
    resulting from




    asymmetric investment theory.






    2.

    Conclusion






    As we can learn from the reviewed

    studies on


    pheromones, the model of humans being only optical

    animals has to be revised. Human sociosexual

    interactions

    are in
    fluenced by

    pheromones, even if they cannot be

    detected consciously. Pheromones have the potential to



    in
    fluence human behaviour and

    physiology and so there has

    to be asked the question, in which way the modern striving

    for cleanliness and

    odourlessness affects our everyday social

    lives and human reproductive success in the future. What we

    know

    at the moment, as many studies in the last few years

    have pointed out, is that the human sense of smell has by

    far

    been underestimated in the past and that humans, like other

    animals, use olfactory signals for the

    transmission of

    biologically relevant

    information.






    References






    [/siz

    e]
    [1] Kohl JV, Atzmueller M, Fink B, Grammer K. Human pheromones:




    integrating neuroendocrinology and ethology. Neuroendocrinol Lett



    2001;22:309
    21.

    [2] Zajonc RB.

    Feeling and thinking: preferences need no inferences. Am

    Psychol

    1980;35:151
    75.

    [3] Karlson P,

    Luscher M.

    Pheromones: a new term for a class of

    biologically active substances.

    Nature 1959;183:55
    6.

    [4]

    McClintock MK. Human pheromones: primers, releasers, signallers or

    modulators? In: Wallen K, Schneider E,

    editors. Reproduction in

    context. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2000. p.

    335
    420.

    [5] Graham JM, Desjardins

    C. Classical conditioning: induction of luteinizing

    hormone and testosterone secretion in anticipation of

    sexual

    activity. Science

    1980;210:1039
    41.

    [6] Cutler WB.

    Human sex-attractant pheromones: discovery, research,

    development, and application in sex therapy. Psychiat

    Ann

    1999;29:54
    9.

    [7] Wysocki

    CJ, Lepri JJ. Consequences of removing the vomeronasal

    organ. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol

    1991;39:661
    9.

    [8] Tirindelli R,

    Mucignat-Caretta C, Ryba NJP. Molecular aspects of

    pheromonal communication via the vomeronasal organ of

    mammals.

    Trends Neurosci

    1998;21:482
    6.

    [9] Halpern M. The

    organization and function of the vomeronasal system.

    Ann Rev Neurosci

    1987;10:325
    62.

    [10] Monti-Bloch

    L, Jennings-White C, Berliner DL. The human

    vomeronasal system: a review. Ann N Y Acad Sci Nov 30



    1998;855:373
    89.

    [11] Smith TD,

    Siegel MI, Mooney MP, Burdi AR, Fabrizio PA, Clemente

    FR. Searching for the vomeronasal organ of adult humans:

    preliminary






    fi
    [size=

    1]ndings on location, structure, and size. Microsc Res Tech




    1998;41:483
    91.

    [12] Grosser BI,

    Monti-Bloch L, Jennings-White C, Berliner DL. Behavioural

    and electrophysiological effects of androstadienone, a

    human

    pheromone. Psychoneuroendocrinology

    2000;25:289
    99.

    [13] Rodriguez I,

    Greer CA, MokMY, Mombaerts P. A putative pheromone

    receptor gene expressed in human olfactory mucosa. Nat

    Genet

    2000;26:18
    9.

    [14]

    Brooksbank BWL, Wilson DAA, MacSweeney DA. Fate of androsta-

    4, 16-dien-3-one and the origin of

    3
    a-hydroxy-5a-androst-16-ene in

    man. J Endocrinol

    1972;52:239
    [size=1]51.






    [/

    size]
    K. Grammer et al. / European Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology and

    Reproductive Biology 118 (2005)

    135
    142

    141


    [15] Gower DB, Ruparelia BA. Olfaction in humans with special

    reference to

    odourous 16-androstenes: their occurrence, perception and possible

    social, psychological and

    sexual impact. J Endocrinol

    1993;137:167
    87.

    [16] Michael RP,

    Bonsall RW, Kutner M. Volatile fatty acids,

    Copulins, in

    human vaginal secretions. Psychoneuroendocrinol

    1975;1:153
    62.

    [17] van der Lee S,

    Boot LM. Spontaneous pseudopregnancy in mice. Acta

    Physiol Pharmacol Nee

    1955;4:442
    3.

    [18] Whitten WK.

    Modi
    fication of the estrous cycle of

    the mouse by

    external stimuli associated with the male. J Endocrinol 1956;13:



    399
    404.

    [19] Bruce HM. An

    exteroceptive block to pregnancy in the mouse. Nature

    1959;184:105.

    [20] Vandenburgh JG. Effect of the

    presence of the male on the sexual

    maturation of female mice. Endocrinology

    1967;81:345
    9.

    [21] Grammer K.

    5-
    a-androst-16en-3a-on: a male pheromone? A brief

    report Ethol Sociobiol

    1993;14:201
    8.

    [22] Miller EM. The

    pheromone androstenol: evolutionary considerations.

    Mankind Q

    1999;39:455
    67.

    [23] McClintock

    MK. Menstrual synchrony and suppression. Nature



    1971;229:244
    5.

    [24] Preti G,

    Cutler WB, Garcia CR, Krieger A, Huggins GR, Lawley HJ.

    Human axillary secretions

    in
    fluence

    women
    s menstrual cycles: the

    role

    of donor extracts of females. Horm Behav

    1986;20:474
    82.

    [25] Cutler WB,

    Preti G, Krieger A, Huggins GR, Garcia CR, Lawley HJ.

    Human axillary secretions

    in
    fluence

    women
    s menstrual cycles: the

    role

    of donor extracts from men. Horm Behav

    1986;20:463
    73.

    [26] Ellis BJ,

    Garber J. Psychosocial antecedents in variation in

    girls







    puber

    tal timing: maternal depression, stepfather presence, and marital


    and family stress. Child Dev

    2000;71:485
    501.

    [27] Kirk-Smith

    M, Booth MA, Carroll D, Davies P. Human social attitudes

    affected by androstenol. Res Comm Psych Psychiat

    Behav

    1978;3:379
    84.

    [28] Van

    Toller C, Kirk-Smith M, Lombard J, Dodd GH. Skin conductance

    and subjective assessments associated with the

    odour of 5
    a-androstan-

    3-one. Biol

    Psychol 1983;16:85
    107.

    [29]

    Benton D, Wastell V. Effects of androstenol on human sexual arousal.

    Biol Psychol

    1986;22:141
    7.

    [30] Filsinger EE,

    Braun JJ, Monte WC. An examination of the effects of

    putative pheromones on human judgements. Ethol Sociobiol



    1985;6:227
    36.

    [31] Doty RL,

    Snyder PJ, Huggins GR, Lowry LD. Endocrine, cardiovascular,

    and psychological correlates of olfactory

    sensitivity changes

    during the human menstrual cycle. J Comp Physiol Psychol



    1981;95:45
    60.

    [32] Benton D.

    The in
    fluence of androstenol, a

    putative human

    pheromone







    o

    n mood throughout the menstrual cycle. Biol Psychol




    1982;15:249
    56.

    [33] Thorne F,

    Neave N, Scholey A, Moss M, Fink B. Effects of putative

    male pheromones on female ratings of male

    attractiveness: in
    fluence

    of oral

    contraception and the menstrual cycle. Neuroendocrinol Lett



    2002;23:291
    7.

    [34] Black SL,

    Biron C. Androstenol as a human pheromone: no effect on

    perceived attractiveness. Behav Neural Biol

    1982;34:326
    30.

    [35] Rogel MJ. A

    critical examination of the possibility of higher prinmate

    reproductive and sexual pheromones. Psych Bull

    1978;85:810
    30.

    [36] Morris NM,

    Udry J. Pheromonal in
    fluences on human

    sexual behaviour:

    an experimental search. J Biosocial Sci

    1978;10:147
    57.

    [37] Cowley JJ,

    Brooksbank BWL. Human exposure to putative pheromones

    and changes in aspects of social behaviour. J Steroid

    Biochem

    Mol Biol 1991;39:647
    59.



    [38] Cutler WB, Friedmann E, McCoy NL. Pheromonal

    in
    fluences on

    sociosexual

    behaviour in men. Arch Sex Behav

    1998;27:1
    13.

    [39] McCoy NL,

    Pitino L. Pheromonal in
    fluences on

    sociosexual behaviour

    in young women. Physiol Behav

    2002;75:367
    75.

    [40] Hamilton WD,

    Zuk M. Heritable true
    fitness and

    bright birds: a role for

    parasites. Science

    1982;218:384
    7.

    [41] Folstad I,

    Karter AJ. Parasites, bright males, and the immunocompetence

    handicap. Am Nat

    1992;139:603
    22.

    [42] Jordan WC,

    Bruford MW. New perspectives on mate choice and the

    MHC. Heredity

    1998;81:239
    45.

    [43] Hurst JL,

    Payne CE, Nevison CM, Marie AD, Humphries RE,

    Robertson DHL. et al. Individual recognition in mice mediated by



    major urinary proteins. Nature

    2001;414:631
    4.

    [44] Alberts SC,

    Ober C. Genetic variability of the MHC: a review of nonpathogen-

    mediated selective mechanisms. Yb Phys

    Anthropol

    1993;36:71
    89.

    [45]

    Brown JL, Eklund A. Kin recognition and the major histocompatibility

    complex: an integrative review. Am Nat

    1994;143:435
    61.

    [46] Wedekind C,

    Seebeck T, Bettens F, Paepke AJ. MHC-dependent mate

    preferences in humans. Proc R Soc Lond B

    1995;260:245
    9.

    [47] Thornhill R,

    Gangstad SW. The scent of symmetry: a human

    sex pheromone that signals

    fitness? Evol Hum Behav 1999;20:



    175
    201.

    [48] Ober C, Weitkamp

    LR, Cox N, Dytch H, Kostyu D, Elias S. HLA and

    mate choice in humans. Am J Hum Genet

    1997;61:497
    504.

    [49] Milinkski M,

    Wedekind C. Evidence for MHC-correlated perfume

    preferences in humans. Behav Ecol

    2001;12:140
    9.

    [50] Trivers RL.

    Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campbell B,

    editor. Sexual selection and the descent of man

    1871
    1971. Chicago:

    Aldine; 1972.

    p. 136.

    [51] Buss DM. Sex differences in human mate

    preferences
    evolutionary



    hypothesis tested in 37 cultures. Behav Brain Sci

    1989;12:1
    49.

    [52] Alexander RD,

    Noonan KM. Concealment of ovulation, parental care,

    and human social evolution. In: Chagnon NA, Irons WG,

    editors.

    Evolutionary biology and human social behavior. Scituate: North

    Duxbury Press; 1979.

    [53]

    Symons D. The evolution of human sexuality. Oxford: Oxford University

    Press; 1979.

    [54] Benshoof L,

    Thornhill R. The evolution of monogamy and concealed

    ovulation in humans. J Soc Biol Struct

    1979;2:95
    106.

    [55] Gray JP,Wolfe

    LD. Human female sexual cycles and the concealment

    of ovulation problem. J Soc Biol Struc

    1983;6:345
    52.

    [56] Strassman B.

    Sexual selection, paternal care, and concealed ovulation

    in humans. Ethol Sociobiol

    1981;2:31
    40.

    [57] Daniels D. The

    evolution of concealed ovulation and self-deception.

    Ethol Sociobiol

    1983;4:87
    96.

    [58] Bellis MA,

    Baker RR. Do females promote sperm-competition? Data

    for humans Anim Behav

    1991;40(5):997
    9.

    [59] Maiworm RE.

    In
    fluence of androstenone,

    androstenol, menstrual

    cycle, and oral contraceptives on the attractivity ratings of female

    probands. Paper

    presented at the 9th Congress of ECRO; 1990.

    [60] Michael RP, Keverne EB. Pheromones in the communication of

    sexual

    status in primates. Nature

    1968;218:746
    9.

    [61] Michael RP,

    Bonsall RW,Warner P. Human vaginal secretions: volatile

    fatty acid content. Science

    1974;186:1217
    9.

    [62] Preti G,

    Huggins GR. Cyclical changes in volatile acidic metabolites

    of human vaginal secretions and their relation to

    ovulation. J Chem

    Ecol

    1975;1(3):361
    76.

    [63] Waltman R,

    Tricom V,Wilson Jr GE, Lewin AH, Goldberg NL, Chang

    MMY. Volatile fatty acids in vaginal secretions: human

    pheromones?

    Lancet 1973;2:496.

    [64] Michael RP. Determinants of primate reproductive behavior. Acta



    endocrinol

    1972;166(Suppl.):322
    61.

    [65]

    Curtis RF, Ballantine JA, Keverne EB, Bonsall RW, Michael RP.



    Identi
    fication of primate sexual

    pheromones and the properties of

    synthetic attractants. Nature

    1971;232:396
    8.

    [66] Cowley JJ,

    Johnson AL, Brooksbank BWL. The effect of two odorous

    compounds on performance in an assessment-of-people test.

    Psychoneuroendocrinology



    1977;2:159
    72.

    [67] Doty RL,

    Ford M, Preti G. Changes in the intensity and pleasentness

    of human vaginal odors during the menstrual cycle.

    Science



    1975;190:1316
    8.







    n
    K. Grammer
    142

    et al. / European Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology and Reproductive Biology

    118 (2005)

    135
    142





    Discussion

    on this topic can be found by clicking

    here.
    Last edited by oscar; 10-29-2005 at 05:34 AM. Reason: Inserting Link

Thread Information

Users Browsing this Thread

There are currently 1 users browsing this thread. (0 members and 1 guests)

Similar Threads

  1. MAJOR SEXUAL HITS BECAUSE OF PHEROMONES
    By johnngo19 in forum Humor
    Replies: 37
    Last Post: 09-08-2006, 07:55 PM
  2. Using Pheromones for Attraction & Bonding -Article
    By Io_Sono in forum Pheromone Discussion
    Replies: 37
    Last Post: 03-09-2005, 07:42 PM
  3. Lily of the Valley and Pheromones
    By Thanatos in forum Women's Forum
    Replies: 22
    Last Post: 07-15-2004, 10:00 AM
  4. Pheromones & Human Sexuality???
    By **DONOTDELETE** in forum Archives 1
    Replies: 4
    Last Post: 11-01-2001, 05:17 PM
  5. Other non sexual pheromones that bond?
    By **DONOTDELETE** in forum Archives 2
    Replies: 5
    Last Post: 06-24-2001, 08:38 AM

Bookmarks

Posting Permissions

  • You may not post new threads
  • You may not post replies
  • You may not post attachments
  • You may not edit your posts
  •